An excerpt from Chapter 6 (Formative Years) from Nature via Nurture by Matt Ridley
The 1990s saw a series of studies that revived the idea of homosexuality as a "biological" rather than a psychological condition, as a destiny rather than a choice. There were studies showing that future homosexuals had different personalities in childhood, studies showing that homosexual men had differences in brain anatomy from heterosexual men, several twin studies showing that homosexuality was highly heritable in western society, and anecdotal reports from homosexual men to the effect that they had felt "different" early in life." On its own none of these studies was overwhelming. But together, and set against decades of proof that aversion therapy, "treatment," and prejudice entirely failed to "cure" people of gay instincts, the studies were emphatically clear. Homosexuality is an early, probably prenatal, and irreversible preference. Adolescence simply throws fuel on the fire.

What exactly is homosexuality? It is plainly a whole range of behavioral characteristics. In some ways gay men seem to be more like women: they are attracted to men, they may pay more attention to clothes, they are often more interested in people than, say, football. In other ways, however, they are more like heterosexual men: they buy pornography and seek casual sex, for example. (Playgirl's nude centerfolds of men turned out to appeal mainly to gays, not the intended women.)

People, like all mammals, are naturally female unless masculinized. Female is the "default sex" (it is the other way around in birds). A single gene, called SRY, on the Y chromosome starts a cascade of events in the developing fetus leading to the development of masculine appearance and behavior. If that gene is absent, a female body results. It is therefore reasonable to hypothesize that homosexuality in men results from the partial failure of this prenatal masculinization process in the brain, though not in the body (see chapter 9).

By far the most reliable discovery about the causes of homosexuality in recent years is Ray Blanchard's theory of the fraternal birth order. In the mid-1990s Blanchard measured the number of elder brothers and sisters of gay men compared with the population average. He found that gay men are more likely to have elder brothers (but not elder sisters) than either gay women or heterosexual men. He has since confirmed this in 14 different samples from many different places. For each extra older brother, a man's probability of being gay rises by one-third. (This does not mean that men with many elder brothers are bound to be gay: an increase from, say, 3 percent of the population to 4 percent is an increase of one-third.)

Blanchard calculates that at least one gay man in seven, probably more, can attribute his sexual orientation to this effect of fraternal birth order. It is not simply birth order, because having elder sisters has no such effect. Something about elder brothers must actually be causing homosexuality in men. Blanchard believes the mechanism is in the womb rather than the family. One clue lies in the birth weight of baby boys who will later become homosexual. Normally, a second baby is heavier than a first baby of the same sex. Boys especially are heavier if they are born after one or more sisters. But boys born after one brother are only slightly heavier than firstborn boys, and boys born after two or more brothers are usually smaller than first-and second-born boys at birth. By analyzing questionnaires given to gay and straight men and their parents, Blanchard was able to show that younger brothers who went on to become homosexual were 170 grams lighter at birth than younger brothers who went on to become heterosexual. He confirmed the same result-high birth order, low birth weight compared with controls-in a sample of 250 boys (with an average age of seven) who were showing sufficient "cross-gender" wishes to have been referred to psychiatrists; cross-gender behavior in childhood is known to predict later homosexuality.

Like Barker, Blanchard believes that conditions in the womb are marking the baby for life. In this case, he argues, something about occupying a womb that has already held other boys occasionally results in reduced birth weight, a larger placenta (presumably in compensation for the difficulty the baby experiences in growing), and a greater probability of homosexuality. That something, he suspects, is a maternal immune reaction. The immune reaction of the mother, primed by the first male fetuses, grows stronger with each male pregnancy. If it is mild, it causes only a slight reduction in birth weight; if strong, it causes a marked reduction in birth weight and an increased probability of homosexuality.

What could the mother be reacting to? There are several genes expressed only in males, and some are already known to raise an immune reaction in mothers. Some are expressed prenatally in the brain. One intriguing new possibility is a gene called PCDH22, which is on the Y chromosome, is therefore specific to males, and is probably involved in building the brain. It is the recipe for a protocadherin (see Chapter 5). Could this be the gene that wires the bit of the brain that is peculiar to males? A maternal immune reaction may be sufficient to prevent the wiring of the part of the brain that would eventually encourage a fascination with female bodies.

Clearly not all homosexuality is caused this way. Some of it may be caused directly by genes in the homosexual person without the mediation of the mother's immune reaction. Blanchard's theory may explain why it has proved so hard to pin down the "gay gene." The main method for finding such a gene is to compare markers on the chromosomes of homosexual men with those of their heterosexual brothers. But if many gay men have straight elder brothers, this method would work poorly. Besides, the key genetic difference might be on the mother's chromosomes, where it causes the immune reaction. This might explain why homosexuality looks as though it is inherited through the female line: genes for a stronger maternal immune reaction could appear to be "gay genes," even though they may not be expressed in the gay man himself but only in the mother.

But notice what this does to nature versus nurture. If nurture, in this case birth order, causes some homosexuality, it does so by causing an immune reaction, which is a process directly mediated by genes. So is this that environmental or genetic? It hardly matters, because the absurd distinction between reversible nurture and inevitable nature has now been well and truly buried. Nurture in this case looks just as irreversible as nature, perhaps more so.

Politically, the confusion is even greater. Most homosexuals welcomed the news in the mid-1990s that their sexual orientation looked "biological." They wanted it to be a destiny, not a choice, because that would undermine the argument of homophobes that it was a choice and therefore morally questionable. How could it be wrong if it was innate? Their reaction is understandable but dangerous. A greater tendency to violence is also innate in the human male. That does not make it right. Reasoning that "ought" can be derived from "is" is called "naturalistic fallacy." To base any moral position on a natural fact, whether that fact is derived from nature or from nurture, is asking for trouble. In my morality, and I hope in yours, some things are bad but natural, like dishonesty and violence; others are good but less natural, like generosity and fidelity.

Counter
Hosted by www.Geocities.ws

1